The Flu Vaccine: A Recap

I admit that last post was kind of intense, all be it a little controversial. A few readers didn't much care for the way I re-evaluated the numbers, even going as far as to say that I was lying. I am aware that not everybody is ready to be un-plugged from the matrix of mainstream medicine, but I certainly don't want anybody to walk away from this blog with a bad taste in their mouth. My goal is to empower you to take control of your health, stimulate thought and discussion, and open your eyes, ears and hearts to different opinions other than your own- not doop you into not getting a vaccine because of what I personally believe. Luckily a little syntax can go a long way, so lets see if I can't clarify and break that last post into bullet points.

  • Looking at the results from the 2012 meta-analysis, the flu vaccine is about 56% effective. In other words, about half as many people get the flu when vaccinated according to the cited studies (4). However, this number does not take into account the numerous confounding variables that likely also contribute to so-called "vaccine effectiveness".

  • However, the flu vaccine decreases incidence of the flu by 1.55%. That is, it lowers the odds of someone getting the flu from 2.73 to 1.18% (4).

  • Researchers estimate that for every 100 people who get the vaccine, ONE case of influenza like illness (ILI) is prevented (10). Keep in mind, ILI may or may not be the actual flu, it simply means that someone feels like the flu.

  • Other research estimates that for every 4,000 elderly people who receive the vaccine, ONE case of influenza related death is prevented (5). It is worth noting that it is the elderly who are most likely to get the flu and also account for approximately 90% of all influenza-related deaths.

  • This is all assuming that confounding variables are not responsible for the benefit seen. However, there is ample evidence that indicates that part or ALL of the benefit seen in vaccination studies is due to variables other than vaccine effectiveness (1,2,4). For more on this, please see my previous article.

  • It has been shown that the flu vaccine doesn't do anything to prevent community acquired pneumonia (9). This is important, because community acquired pneumonia causes approximately 34,000 of the 36,000 yearly deaths associated with the flu.

 
I would also like to point out that the flu vaccine is not benign. It, like anything else, can do harm. When you get a flu shot you are getting injected with things like mercury, formaldehyde, and aluminum salts (11). Just like we should look at the label for the foods we eat, so should we at least look at the ingredients of the vaccines we inject into our bodies. If you saw a food with formaldehyde in it would you still eat it? Would you feed it to your child?

Lastly, have you ever noticed the frequency with which the flu vaccine seems to make people sick? I'm not saying that it gives you the flu, but it seems like a lot of people just don't feel good after they get the flu shot. I know that word of mouth hardly has the street-cred of a meta-analysis, but its still something. After all, it was only simple hearsay that warned our ancestors to not eat the poisonous "death cap" mushroom a few thousand years ago... Imagine what would have happened if nobody had listened back then! If only Piers Morgan had listened to theflu vaccine nay-sayers..... (12)
I'm not saying we shouldn't try to prevent the flu. Nobody wants to get the flu, and it can be a serious illness. I just don't think that the flu shot is the safest, most effective or most logical way to do it. Instead of taking a disease-centered approach to care, I think that we'd have a better shot at preventing diseases if we focused on our health. For that matter, even if you do choose to get the flu shot I encourage you to still take an active roll in your health. Don't simply rely on the supposed passive immunity from the vaccine to save you, but take every step you can to ensure your whole-body health this flu season.


In health,

Nikki


References:
(1-10) Please see references from my previous post
(11) http://www.cdc.gov/vaccines/parents/vaccine-decision/ingredients.html
(12) http://2012thebigpicture.wordpress.com/2013/01/27/piers-morgan-falls-ill-days-after-public-flu-shot-with-dr-oz/

The Flu Vaccine: Your Best Shot at Preventing the Flu?

Once again, the flu season is well underway and there is no shortage of commercials, posters, billboards, and doctors telling us to get our flu shot. But is the flu shot really your best shot at preventing the flu? In part 1 of this series on flu prevention I hope to prove to you that the flu vaccine isn't as impressive as we have been led to believe. In the next post(s) I will discuss what has been proven in the research to naturally prevent the flu.
I know you're not all scientists, but a quick overview of the different forms of scientific research is necessary to fully appreciate the second half of this article. There are many types of research articles out there, and they all have varying degrees of abundance and credibility in the scientific community. For a more detailed description and explanation please see this website. For now all you need to know is that not all research is created equal. The most credible is the meta-analysis, followed by systemic reviews, randomized double-blind placebo studies, then cohort studies. Most of the research on the flu vaccine tends to be cohort studies, which are decent, but can still have a lot of flaws. Randomized control trials eliminate a lot of bias, but may still have issues as we will discuss below. Meta-analyses are considered to be the best because of their rigorous inclusion criteria. These studies compile information from many different articles, and more importantly, only the best articles make it into a meta-analysis.

As you will soon see, there are many different issues surrounding the debate of Influenza Vaccine Effectiveness. Issues like cohort (study group) selection bias, non-specific end points, and proper diagnosis of the flu can all stand in the way of an otherwise decent study, and will undoubtedly alter the reported numbers. Because it seems to be the most commonly recognized confounding variable in cohort studies, I will focus briefly on selection bias.

The term "selection bias" is a little bit of a misnomer in this case, because it is seen in cohort studies where the groups are not actually "selected" by the scientists. Rather, these studies follow two groups of people (those who got the vaccine and those who did not) and use the information to estimate how effective the vaccine is. The problem is that it's nearly impossible to account for other differences between the two groups of people. Factors such as health status, belief or disbelief in vaccine effectiveness, vaccine availability and possibly other unknown variables may all lead to the difference in behavior studied (i.e. the behavior to get the vaccine or to choose to not be vaccinated).

A great deal of skeptisism comes from what scientists are calling the "healthy user" effect, which means that the people who are getting vaccinated tend to be healthier, and therefore less likely to get the flu or die from it, than those who do not get the vaccine. This is the image of the ideal patient- someone who listens to their doctors advice; weather it be to eat more fiber, exercise, or get a vaccine [4]. One study examined this more in depth and compared several variables between the two groups: age, sex, the log of the insurance risk score, self-assessed health status, and presence of chronic diseases such as diabetes and heart disease. This study found a strong correlation between insurance risk score and self-assessed health status with risk of death, but more importantly they found a strong negative correlation between risk of death (poorer health prior to the flu season) and vaccine coverage. In other words, the more frail people, those who were the most likely to die with or without the flu, were among the least likely to get the flu shot, which undoubtedly skews the results of cohort studies in favor of higher vaccine effectiveness [5].

Aside from the patient's conscious choice there are also other factors that may create this effect. For example, vaccination is generally not recommended for the extremely frail or immuno-comprimised, a group that would be expected to have naturally higher hospitalization and death rates, who then get lumped into the "unvaccinated" group in a simple cohort study [4]. This is called a "frailty selection" [1]. In placebo controlled studies, it is considered unethical to include high-risk patients in such studies (it's not nice to give someone a placebo when there is a higher likelihood that the vaccine can save their life), so there is absolutely no high-quality (double blind, placebo controlled) data on the group that is theorized to benefit from vaccination the most [2].

In their paper, Simonsen et al [1] outlined criteria that can be used to detect such bias. The authors note, however, that it is easier to detect bias than to account for it and calculate exactly how much of the results are due to bias.

Seasonality. Think about it, the vaccine's ability to prevent death should be seasonal- only during the flu season when the virus is actually circulating. If there is a benefit outside of the flu season when the virus is not circulating, this is a strong indicator of a difference between the two groups. Several studies have identified a similar effect on all-cause mortality and hospitalization both during flu season and the off season, indicating an intrinsic difference between the ultimately immunized and non-immunized cohorts [1,6,7]. In 2008 Eurich et al demonstrated a 51% reduction in all cause mortality in pneumonia patients outside the flu season in those who would eventually get the flu shot- a number that is strikingly similar to what other groups report during the flu season [6]. The authors go on to say that this indicates that some or all of the benefit in mortality that is seen in cohort studies is likely due to confounding variables such as the healthy user effect [1].

Vaccine match. Each year the flu virus mutates, creating the supposed need to get the updated yearly flu shot. Each summer and fall, scientists have to try to predict which flu virus will be the predominant strain to make a vaccine against it. Some seasons end up being better matches than others. Such information can be found on the CDC website [8]. Logically, the effectiveness of the vaccine should fluctuate with how well the vaccine is matched to the circulating strain. However, this does not appear to be the case [1,4]. Below is a chart from a 2012 meta-analysis (remember, this means that the studies they use should be the best of the best) that shows how well the vaccine is matched and effectiveness [1]. As you can see, most of the studies from the years that were reported as being mismatched were reported to have the same or better effectiveness than other years. How can that possibly be?
The use of specific end points is important for obtaining accurate, clean results in a study. The more specific your end point (the thing you're measuring), the better the data you can obtain. Three things that are commonly used to describe flu vaccine effectiveness are all cause mortality, Influenza-like illness (someone says they have the flu), and laboratory confirmed influenza. Since people die from a lot of different things, even during the flu season, we would expect the vaccine's effectiveness at preventing all cause mortality to be relatively low. On the flip side, since the specific goal of the vaccine is to prevent the real flu (as opposed to ILI which may be the flu virus or something else), we would expect the vaccine's effectiveness here to be highest. On the contrary, current cohort evidence shows vaccine effectiveness estimates are highest (and implausibly large) for all cause mortality and lowest for laboratory confirmed influenza [1].

Cohort studies are most prone to bias because of the healthy-user effect, but even placebo controlled trials have their problems. Not only is the specificity of the end point being measured a concern, but the way those results are obtained may be an issue even in placebo controlled studies. Serology confirmed or laboratory confirmed influenza has been reported to skew results in favor of exaggerating vaccine effectiveness [2,4]. Culture confirmed influenza or better, vaccine-matched culture confirmed influenza seem to be the best in this regard, but the research in this area is sparse. Most studies look at all cause mortality, and many of the others look at laboratory confirmed infection, neither of which will give entirely accurate results.

Several authors have noted that the repeatedly reported 50% effectiveness against all cause mortality is simply not realistic. The flu only accounts for about 5% of all deaths in the winter, so "that the influenza vaccine can prevent ten times as many deaths as the disease itself causes is not plausible"[1]. Because influenza only contributes a small percentage to all cause mortality, it cannot be reasonably expected to do more than eliminate this excess. As I will show below, the real flaw lies in how that number, 50% is used, but there is another point to be made here. Several studies in the United States have tried to evaluate weather there has been a decrease in all cause mortality with increasing vaccination rates. In the last twenty years, since the flu vaccination rate among the elderly has soared from 15% to 65% there has not been a single study to has documented any change in hospital admission rates nor all cause mortality [6]. Similarly, studies failed to show an increase in mortality during the 1997-98 season, during which the vaccine was completely mismatched with the circulating strain [1].

Okay, here comes the really crazy part of this whole mess. For the time being, let's put aside all the stuff I just talked about and focus on the numbers- we'll assume that the data the studies are getting is actually reflective of what the vaccine is doing and that none of it is from confounding variables. We're not all statisticians here, but I think we can all do some basic 8th grade math. Let's solve this word problem:

100 people get the flu vaccine. Two of them get the flu. 100 other people do not get the vaccine. Four of them get the flu. How effective is the vaccine?

Okay, pencils down. Most of us would think through this problem like so, "We need to compare what percentage of each group got the flu. 2 percent versus 4 percent of each group got the flu. The vaccine was therefore 2% effective at preventing the flu. In other words, for every 100 people who got the flu vaccine, two less got the flu." What we just did was we calculated the absolute difference between the two groups- the percent difference. This is how people generally perceive percentages- we like to think of the number of people who's life would have been changed with the intervention. However, this is not how vaccine effectiveness is calculated. These studies calculate (and then report to us, the lay people) the relative difference between the two numbers. This is what it would be like for the example above, "We need to compare what percentage of each group got the flu. 2 percent versus 4 percent got the flu. 4 is twice as big as 2. The vaccine is therefore 50% effective."  Let that logic soak in for a minute or so. Yes, this is a perfectly legitimate way to compare these two numbers, two and four, but exactly what that percentage is comparing is rarely, if ever explained on flu vaccine posters. No wonder they're (and we) all are getting so confused! I think this is incredibly misleading. Let's look at the numbers from the studies cited by that 2012 meta-analysis again and compare the absolute values to their inflated relative values:

The way I see it, there has been an unintentional miscommunication between the science (research) world and the consumer world. We tend to see things how they relate to ourselves and we like to apply numbers to real life. I think when people see that the flu vaccine is 50% effective, they imagine it as "if I was 100% at risk, now that risk is only 50% which is pretty good". The reality is that most people's odds of getting the flu are already pretty low (2.73% odds without the vaccine if we use the numbers cited above), especially if you take care of yourself. What the vaccine does is it lowers your risk of getting the flu from 2.73% to 1.18%- which to you is only an overall difference in risk of 1.55%. Another way of looking at it is that you need to vaccinate 100 people to prevent ONE set of influenza symptoms [10]. As if that's not impressive enough, Fireman et al estimated that you would need to vaccinate 4,000 people to prevent one death from the flu in their elderly Kaiser Permanente cohort [5]. Again, this is the group of people theorized to need the vaccine the most and whom make up approximately 90% of influenza related deaths. Oh, and let's not forget that this doesn't even take into consideration the numerous confounding variables we talked about in the first half of the article, some of which have been shown to either partially or completely explain any benefit the vaccine has been shown to have. Call me crazy, but that doesn't sound nearly as impressive as the 50-70% effectiveness rates we typically hear about...

Not only is this misleading to the lay person, but researchers seem to be confused by their own statistics. Recall if you will, the authors who said that the 50% reduction in all cause mortality reported with the vaccine was unrealistic because the flu itself only accounts for about 5% of all winter deaths. They are trying to compare apples to oranges- you would have to back track and look at the absolute differences in order to compare these results to all cause mortality. Perhaps if they used the numbers from the Fireman et al study, 1 death prevented for every 4,000 vaccinated, that would make more sense. The kicker is that this wording was only used once toward the end of the article. If you read their abstract or their conclusion, however, even this group reported the vaccine effectiveness rate as 47% [5]!

It is worth noting that numerous studies have demonstrated that the vaccine is least effective in the two groups that are presumed to need it the most- children and the elderly [1,4].  In one trial the researchers estimate that the vaccine effectiveness drops from 75% to 23% between the ages of 65 and 70- a huge and important drop considering that 90% of all influenza deaths are in folks over the age of 65. There were no randomized clinical control trials that were good enough to meet inclusion criteria for the 2012 meta-analysis that showed TIV (trivalent influenza vaccine) effectiveness in people aged 2-17 or over 65 [4]. Only the live attenuated vaccine (LAIV) was shown to have any effect in the 65 years old plus crowd, but that vaccine is not approved for use in adults over 50 in the United States [4]. Perhaps most importantly for seniors, the influenza vaccine has been shown to do nothing to prevent community acquired pneumonia. Pneumonia is responsible for the vast majority of influenza-related deaths (approximately 34,000 of the 36,000 deaths) each year [9], so this certainly doesn't lend much credibility to those estimates on decreasing all cause mortality.


I think it's safe to say at this point that there are a LOT of things to consider when it comes to this body of research. There are numerous confounding variables that may either partially or completely account for the reported effectiveness of the vaccine, "effectiveness" can mean anything from preventing death to preventing the flu, there is a lack of solid evidence that the vaccine works at all in the populations that supposedly need it the most, and the method of reporting the vaccine effectiveness is misleading at best. There are other issues that simply could not make it into this post such as the ingredients in the vaccine, side effects, politics, marketing, and conflicts of interest with funding sources. But all things said, I can not legally tell you whether or not you should get the flu shot. Whether you choose to get the vaccine or not is your personal choice, and I understand that there are a lot of factors that do, and should, go into this decision. All I can hope to do is educate you so that you can make the best decision for you and your family. I personally think I have a better shot (pun!) at preventing the flu if I keep myself healthy... especially when you take the unimpressive results from the research into consideration.

Even after all that, we still haven't gotten to talk about what actually causes the flu! I know what you're thinking, the influenza virus causes the flu. But the reality is that it is predominantly your health as host that determines weather or not a virus will successfully make you sick. Surely we have all lived with someone who has gotten sick, yet did not get sick ourselves. There had to have been a reason why you didn't get sick, and it surely wasn't from lack of exposure. Next blog post we're going to talk about what actually causes the flu, as well as what natural things you can do to prevent it. Spoiler alert- there is a vitamin out there that kicks flu vaccine butt at preventing the flu!


Stay healthy,

Nikki Cyr


References:
[1] Simonsen, L. Mortality benefits of influenza vaccination in elderly people: an ongoing controversy. Lancet Infectious Dis 2007;7:658-66
[2] Jackson, L. Safety, efficacy and immunogenicity of an inactivated influenza vaccine in healthy adults: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial over two influenza seasons. British Medical Journal Infectious Diseases 2010, 10:71
[3] Lang, P. Effectiveness of influenza vaccine in aging and older adults: comprehensive analysis of the evidence. Clinical Interventions in Aging 2012:7; 55-64
[4] Osterholm, M. Efficacy and effectiveness of influenza vaccines: a systemic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Infectious Disease 2012; 12: 36-44
[5] Fireman, B. Influenza vaccination and mortality: Differentiating vaccine effects from bias. American Journal of Epidemiology 2009;170:650-656
[6] Eurich D. Mortality reduction with influenza vaccine in patients with pneumonia outside "flu" season. American Journal of Respiratory Care Medicine 2008;178:527-533
[7] Hottes, T. Influenza vaccine effectiveness in the elderly based on administrative databases: change in immunization habit as a marker for bias. PLoS ONE 6970: e22618
[8] http://www.cdc.gov/flu/pastseasons/1112season.htm
[9]  Jackson. Influenza vaccination and risk of community acquired pneumonia in immunocompetent elderly people: a population-based nested case-control study. 2008;372:398-405
[10] Jefferson, T. Vaccination for preventing influenza in healthy adults. Cochrane database of systemic reviews; 8 article CDOO4879

Fructose, a not-so-sweet surprise?

Fructose has earned itself a special spot in the lime-light in the last ten years, and with good reason. One minute you hear fructose is bad, the next you see a commercial saying it's the same as sugar. One day you go to your alternative medicine doctor and they say stay away from the stuff, the next day you do to your medical doctor and they tell you to use agave nectar because it's low glycemic. It's all very confusing! Well, I'm here to help you sort out the facts, the myths, and most importantly, the marketing.

First of all, let's compare fructose to it's cousins, glucose and galactose. Each of these are monosaccharides (one-unit sugars) that contain 6 carbons. These are bound together to form disaccharides (two-unit sugars). Sucrose, table sugar, is a glucose bound to a galactose. Lactose is a glucose bound to a galactose. These are both so called "simple sugars" because only one bond needs to be broken to get back to sugar building blocks, monosaccharides. Starch, like the glycogen in muscle and liver cells, is made up of long strands of glucose molecules bound together for easy storage. Each of these bonds requires slightly different enzymes for digestion, but the end product that makes its way to your cells for energy is going to be one of these three basic sugars.
Fructose is naturally found in fruit, and is by no means an evil sugar all by itself. However, the invention of high fructose corn syrup (HFCS) in the 1970s has made fructose an all too common occurrence in the American diet today. While the Corn Refiners of America may try to bamboozle us into thinking that HFCS is "a natural sugar that comes from corn" and "like sugar, is fine in moderation", I'm here to tell you that these statements could not be further from the truth.

In terms of their caloric value fructose and glucose are practically the same, but there are several things that make fructose a completely different beast in regards to its effects on the human body.

1. The metabolism of fructose is quite different than glucose.  For you biochemistry nerds out there, in his YouTube video "Sugar: The Bitter Truth" Dr. Lustig details the differences between glucose and fructose metabolism [1]. The two main differences (aside from the hormonal interactions) is that almost all of the metabolic burden of fructose falls onto the liver, and that calorie-for-calorie fructose is more readily converted to fat.

2. Fructose affects the body, and it's hormones differently than glucose. Glucose requires the hormone insulin to get into cells, which triggers adipose (fat) cells to release a hormone called leptin. Leptin tells the brain "I've had enough", thus serving as a negative feedback mechanism to prevent over-consumption of sugar. These hormones also suppress the release of another hormone, ghrelin. Ghrelin is a hunger stimulating hormone that is produced in the stomach. Suppressing ghrelin while increasing leptin secretion gives you the feeling of satiety. However, fructose does none of these things, leaving you yearning for more sugar.

3. Perhaps most importantly, marketing for HFCS and fructose containing products such as agave nectar have lead people to believe that they are actually healthy. The Corn Refiners "sweet surprise" commercials [2] and print ads surely paint a convincing picture. By portraying the HFCS skeptic as a uninformed scare-mongers, who after being questioned by what can only be called the HFCS enthusiast can only shrug and mumble, the Corn Refiners have made it quite clear that you should feel silly for simply questioning their product [2]. I much prefer this spoof commercial [3].
Agave nectar is unique in that many well intentioned doctors are now recommending it as a sugar alternative to their diabetic patients. Touting it's shiny low glycemic index label proudly, agave has managed to find itself a nice little niche among those who have diabetes and a desire to follow a low sugar diet, but still crave sugar. The problem is that agave is NOT low sugar- it is low(er) in glucose. According to my searches, agave ranges anywhere from 70-85% fructose, as opposed to HFCS that is typically 55% fructose, or table sugar that is 50% fructose. The reason agave is low glycemic is that fructose does nothing to raise your blood sugar, because "blood sugar" actually means "blood glucose". When blood glucose raises, diabetics have to take insulin (see number 2). Since fructose does not interact with these hormones it is considered better for diabetics, but that could not be further from the truth. The problem is that they fail to tell you on that nice, shiny label that high fructose sweeteners such as agave will leave you wanting more sugar, is more readily converted by fat, places undue burden on the liver, and is, calorie for calorie, still a sugar.

The moral of the story yet again is that you can't trick the body. We should all be on a low sugar diet-  but this means all sugars and all sweeteners. Don't fool yourself any longer- there is no such thing as a healthy sweetener. I'm not saying don't enjoy your sweets every now and then, but sugar is truly something to be consumed in MODEST moderation.
In a previous post we talked about how artificial sweeteners like aspartame create a feeling of disconnect between the stomach and the brain which leaves you craving more sugar, in addition to the very real possibility of causing cancer [4]. Now we have learned that low glycemic sweeteners (aka high-fructose sweeteners) such as agave and HFCS not only leave you craving more sugar, but will make you more fat and stress your liver. I hope you use this information to make the healthiest possible choices in the future.


Too legit to quit,

Nikki Cyr, D.C.


Referenced links:
[1] https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=dBnniua6-oM  <-- More on the metabolism of fructose
[2] https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=lQ-ByUx552s  <--- Commercial funded by the Corn Refiners
[3] https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hYiEFu54o1E  <--- The real deal!
[4] http://createanewbox.blogspot.com/2012/08/artificially-sweet-part-1-truth-about.html

Other good reads:
[5] http://www.foodrenegade.com/agave-nectar-good-or-bad/
[6] http://articles.mercola.com/sites/articles/archive/2010/01/02/highfructose-corn-syrup-alters-human-metabolism.aspx
[7] http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2008/12/081209221742.htm




My 2013 Goals

In an attempt to follow my own advise, I decided to post my goals for 2013 here for all to see. Some are bigger or smaller than others. Some I already do anyway but I want to make a mental note to keep doing them. Some will be challenging.
One (the work out routine) is an old friend whom I have yet to conquer, but I believe I have identified the reasons why I have failed in the past and have tailored a plan to work around that. I know it's the oldest one in the book, but I have often found myself using the old excuse of not having time to work out. Solution: Work out at home (mostly) and fit mini-work outs in when I walk Ralphy in the morning (something I already do anyway). The other issue I have run into since I stopped rowing is boredom. I was a personal trainer for a year, I have a bachelors in Exercise Science, I am a chiropractor, and I still get bored working out. I know more about the movement I do than any one human being needed to know about movement, and yet, I mentally yawn just thinking about going to the gym. I rowed in college and ran and lifted weights for cross training, but that extra stuff was all because of my love for rowing. I just wanted to be a good rower. Well, that and I knew I had to go to the boathouse every morning or Rudy would probably find me and hurt me. I think I need a workout that 1. Keeps me interested 2. keeps me accountable or 3. Is rowing. Here are my ideas so far.

1. Groupons are a fabulous way to explore what is available in your area AND keep your workout boredom at bay! I bought three workout related groupons right before the new year, which will give me a great sample-platter of workouts to keep me interested. I really enjoyed the first class I went to and will most likely continue to work out there after the groupon runs out : )

2. As of last night, I have officially recruited a workout buddy! Involving friends is a great way to make working out fun AND keep you accountable. I make a really good workout buddy (I have a lot of toys and experiance putting together workout plans), but I make a lousy solo-work outer.

3. My ultimate goal is to join the boat house here in Tempe so I can get back on the water again. I think if I can get myself in decent shape and start rowing again, it will help me keep my eyes on the prize when I do non-rowing workouts.


Practicing what she preaches writes about,

Nikki Cyr, D.C.